Self-Esteem, Language Acculturation and the Mexican-American Immigrant

Home
Little Heaven artwork (Cielito)
Graduate School Papers
My Resume
AVON & SHAKLEE
Trixie Belden Fan Fiction
Inspirations & Chuckles
Favorite Links
About Me/ Contact Me
Family & Friends

 

ABSTRACT

Herein, issues of self-esteem and acculturation are addressed in regards to the Hispanic American population within the United States with particular attention paid to the Mexican-American immigrants. Further, known data regarding this select subgroup of the American population is discussed in conjunction with issues of bicultural living and its effect on the Mexican-Americans level of self-esteem, as well as on family harmony and overall physical and psychological health. Theories of acculturation are briefly discussed with the concept of language acculturation being introduced and expanded upon. Ultimately, a suggestion for a future research proposal is presented regarding the combination of Mexican-American immigrant language acculturation and its effects on an individual’s self-esteem.

 

INTRODUCTION

The United States is currently experiencing an influx of immigrants akin to the historic boom at the beginning of the 20th century. Moreover, one quarter of foreign-born U.S. residents now live in the New York and Los Angeles metropolitan areas. As these families become part of our communities, schools, and society, the need to understand how they adapt to U.S. lifestyle and the problems they encounter becomes ever more apparent. Recent research suggests that acculturation and ethnic identity are influenced by a variety of factors…(Farver, Narang, & Ghadha, 2002, p. 346).

Within the last 20 years, the population within North America has diversified extensively from the once predominantly European descent at the beginning of the 20th century to now being a continent full of Japanese, Filipino, Indians, Hispanics, Chinese, Koreans, and other groups (Gurung & Mehta, 2001). The Hispanic population is one of the fastest growing groups within the United States with reportedly more than 32.8 million Hispanics living in the U.S. in 2002 (Sharma & Kerl, 2002). In reality, Hispanics constitute the second largest minority group living in the United States today with its children being the nation’s youngest subpopulation with1 out of every 5 children in the U.S. being an immigrant child (Negy & Snyder, 1997; McWhirter, Hackett, & Bandalos, 1998; Lau, McCabe, Yeh, Garland, Wood, & Hough, 2005). In 2002, Mexican Americans accounted for 61% of the total Hispanic population within the country with more than 7 million Mexican immigrants residing within its borders in 2003; this number decreased slightly with only 58% of the Hispanic population consisting of Mexican immigrants in the following year (Sharma & Kerl, 2002; Sue & Sue, 2003; Castillo, Conoley, & Brossart, 2004). Government census projections believe that the number of immigrants filling the U.S. population will grow from 10% today to being more than 15% of this country’s inhabitants by 2020 (Sharma & Kerl, 2002).

According to Phinney, Chavira, and Williamson (1992), as the racial and ethnic makeup of the United States continues to become more diverse, the process of acculturation will likely bring conflicts and stress to the people within these growing minority groups, as well as members of the dominant American society. More and more research are showing that acculturative stress is a risk factor for mental health problems, such as self-esteem and self-identity, for immigrants (Lau, McCabe, Yeh, Garland, Wood, & Hough, 2005). As it is, some of the regular problems that Hispanic immigrants face today are high unemployment, 30 percent of Latino children live without health insurance, there is an overrepresentation of Hispanics in substandard housing, a grand number of Latinos are suffering from obesity, AIDS, and tuberculosis, as well as Latinos being the ethnic group with the highest out-of-wedlock births within the country (Sue & Sue, 2003). All of these problems are effected by Hispanic Americans understanding of the dominant English language which can best be described by these words by Baptist:

Significant numbers of immigrants, adults and children, arrive in American with minimal to no fluency in English. Because of the inability to communicate effectively in English, such immigrants, especially the adults, are restricted in their involvement with the society and its institutions. As a result, they remain marginally involved in the society because they rarely, if ever, improve their English language fluency” (Baptiste, 1993, p. 346).

Research has shown that immigrants tend to deny the existence of transitional problems within their families from emigrating, probably because of the fact that they are focused most on surviving in the new environment (Baptiste, 1993). Yet studies have indicated that a satisfactory adjustment to the new environment with time is most probable when the family’s expectations of the United States matches their eventual reality; sadly, though, most families look to the move with high optimism and expectations while ignoring negative information about the new place so that they experience difficulties when they emigrate to the foreign land without knowing the language well (Baptiste, 1993).

Self-esteem is a topic that is spoken of much in the field of psychology for its major effects on all aspects of human life, no matter the age of the person in question. Many psychologists believe that a strong self-esteem is key to enjoying a happy and healthy life (Twenge & Campbell, 2001). Social psychology has shown that a person’s self-concept, originating from personal introspection in conjunction with one’s culture and life experiences, has a strong affect on one’s level of self-esteem (Brehm, Kassin, & Fein, 2005). How a person believes others view him- or herself can also have a great effect on personal self-esteem. In truth, self-esteem has been found to be such a powerful agent that it is said to be the best predictor of psychological and physical health per life satisfaction for Americans (Crocker & Wolfe, 2001).

Thus, it is the intent of this author to provide an overview of completed research regarding both self-esteem and acculturation issues for Mexican-American immigrants. This topic addresses a growing need within the United States, and though some research has been completed to date, there is much more information that needs to be acquired and studied regarding this part of our population. For this reason, a research proposal for a new study, apparently never researched before, will be proposed at the conclusion of this paper.

SELF-ESTEEM

Self-esteem has been described variously as a predictor of human behavior,

a cue to how others react to us, and a fundamental human motive. Moreover, there is substantial belief and moderate evidence that self-esteem is an index of psychological well-being. Although increased attention is being devoted to the assessment of specific facets of self-esteem, self-esteem is most often considered a general judgment of personal worth or value (Gray-Little & Hafdahl, 2000, p.29).

Self-esteem has been negatively correlated with both hopelessness and depression (Phinney, Cantu, Kurtz, 1997). Self-esteem is skewed by the manner one evaluates his or her own thoughts, behaviors, and feelings (Brehm, Kassin, & Fein, 2005). Life stress has been shown to erode one’s feelings of personal control and thus lower one’s self-esteem (Krause, Bennett, & Van Tran, 1989). Thus, it would be reasonable that the stress of moving to a foreign country and not knowing the dominant language would negatively affect one’s self-esteem. Furthermore, De, Carsten, & van Knipperberg (2005) have stated that conflict in life is threatening to one’s self-concept, or the manner in which a person views him- or herself. In truth, accurate self-perception has been dubbed as particularly important to the psychological health of a being while low self-esteem has been linked to persons with poor health (Hoza, et. al., 2004; Wood, Heimpel, Newby-Clark, & Ross, 2005). Conversely, persons who possess high self-esteem experience less anxiety, helplessness, and depression with better health, more positive emotions and an overall greater satisfaction with his or her life (Crocker & Wolfe, 2001; Twenge & Campbell, 2001).

The overall esteem a person has in his- or herself in comparison to others is referred to as global self-esteem (Schmitt & Allik, 2005). In order to meet the expectations of other people, persons will present themselves in one of two manners, either through self-verification (trying to make others see oneself as one sees him- or herself) or strategic self-presentation which involves selling oneself to others in order to become more powerful, better liked, or to receive a promotion of some aspect from others (Brehm, Kassin, & Fein, 2005). The two ways that people use self-presentation is through either self-deceptive enhancement or via impression management (Pauls & Crost, 2005). According to the self-verification theory, people choose to interact with other persons who see him or her as he or she sees him- or herself (Cassidy, Aikins, & Chernoff, 2003). And Gest, Domitrovich, and Welsh (2005) believe that the people one chooses to interact with socially can influence one’s social self-concept.

Ethnic Identity

According to Gurung and Mehta (2001), a strong group identification and ethnic pride are critical for developing positive self-esteem and self-efficacy for persons in the minority. Achieving such a strong self-identity through group affiliation has been shown to be particularly important to adolescents in ethnic groups within the United States (Farver, Narang, & Ghadha, 2002). When persons identify themselves as a member of a group, they take on the beliefs, standards, and rules of the group which works to maintain or bolster self-esteem (Gurung & Mehta, 2001). However, when people are exposed to new social groups, they experience a time of re-categorizing themselves to find a way to merge somehow with this new social group (Gramzow & Gaertner, 2005). This author is sure that this re-categorization is doubly hard for persons who newly move to an area, such as in the lives of Mexican-American immigrants.

The Social Identity Theory

In 1986, Taifel and Turner introduced to the field of Social Psychology the principal of the Social Identity Theory (Phinney, Cantu & Kurtz, 1997). This principal states that each person tries to boost their self-esteem with many in-group affiliations and by enhancing their personal identity through one’s achievements (Brehm, Kassin, & Fein, 2005). Phinney, Cantu, & Kurtz (1997) state that this theory would make one believe that affiliation with such a powerful country as the United States would pump up self-esteem for different ethnic groups but they found in their studies of both African- and Mexican-American students that this theory holds true only to the point that being an “American” is important to each individual. Their research further showed that in-group affiliation with an ethnic minority group can be sorely hurt by how the overarching American society views that group; this is a negative fact for Mexican-Americans who have very negative stereotypes in this country (Phinney, Cantu, & Kurtz, 1997).

Group Categorizations

In-group categorizations such as cultural group, faith affiliation, or gender group can bolster self-esteem just by one being able to claim membership to the group itself and creating feelings of “us” and “them” which can detach the pain of negative statements made by out-groups for one’s memberships as it is only “their” opinions and not “ours” (Conway & Schaller, 2005; Brehm, Kassin, & Fein, 2005). This detachment has been shown to be a powerful deterrent for discrimination and prejudice in the United States for the different minority groups, such as the Mexican-Americans, with one’s identity as a minority group member being quite important to both self-image and global self-esteem (Brehm, Kassin, & Fein, 2005; Phinney, Cantu, & Kurtz, 1997).

Gender Effects on Self-Esteem

There is a definite gender difference in some aspects of male and female self-esteem (Brehm, Kassin, & Fein, 2005). For example, boys tend to find more esteem in their physical prowess while girls seem to place more importance on their social popularity and social relationships (Shapka & Keating, 2005; Gest, Domitrovich, & Welsh, 2005). Though the difference of esteem is not substantial between the sexes, there are transitions that each group seems to experience with girls’ esteem lowering during junior high and raising progressively from then through the college years and boys esteem increasing most during the high school years (Twenge & Campbell, 2001). Wood, Heimpel, Newby-Clark, and Ross (2005) recently stated their hypothesis that success is a tough experience for women as females tend to worry about how to maintain that level of success experienced. Additionally, Scharts, Waldo, and Daniel (2005) completed studies that showed that men who were abusive in their relationships during life were typically suffering from low self-esteem and gender-role conflicts.

Racial Group Affiliation and Self-Esteem

Beyond gender, racial group affiliation has been shown to directly affect one’s self-esteem. For example, a meta-analysis by Twenge and Crocker (2002) showed that different races have different levels of esteem overall, with generally the racial groups having the highest to lowest self-esteem levels in the following order: Blacks in America have the highest esteem followed by Whites with Asian- and Latino-Americans following afterwards with the lowest overall racial self-esteem. Mexican Americans have been found to have the lowest esteem beyond all other groups in the country (Twenge & Crocker, 2002), making research regarding this subgroup within the U.S. population quite necessary. Rather interesting is the fact that Blacks living in the most persecuted part of the U.S., in the American South, were shown to have the highest self-esteem of all racial groups, with positive racial identity believed to be the protective barrier to prejudice‘s negative effects (Brehm, Kassin, & Fein, 2005). Of all the aforementioned groups, men of the Asian-, Hispanic-, and African-American groups reportedly have less self-esteem than White females (Twenge & Crocker, 2002). However, a positive fact is that ratings of self-esteem have been increasing steadily since the 1960s across all races (Twenge & Campbell (2001).

Racial self-esteem consists of how one evaluates members of his or her racial group in difference to persons of other races (Gray-Little & Hafdahl, 2000). Twenge and Crocker’s (2002) meta-analysis further revealed that minority status, positive racial identity, and one’s culture all can effect individual self-esteem. A positive racial identity is believed to act as a wall to the harsher negative effects of racial affiliation, such as stereotypes and prejudice (Gray-Little & Hafdahl, 2000). Research by both Twenge and Crocker (2002) support this theory, yet the majority of research has been completed on Black-White differences, indicating that other subgroups like Mexican-Americans need to be further studied on the subject of positive racial identity.

Faith and Self-Esteem

Crocker and Wolfe (2001) postulate that some minority groups, such as African Americans or Latinos, place more importance on God’s love and acceptance for their persons to decide on their self-worth than on the beliefs of other people they know. Cauce et al. (2002) support this belief as persons from minority groups often use prayer and spirituality to cope with problems and social persecution such as prejudice.

Socioeconomic Status and Self-Esteem

The level of socioeconomic status (SES) that a person endures has been shown to effect self-esteem with White, Black, and Hispanic-Americans who lived in the lower SES groups of the United States repeatedly reporting lower self-esteem than those persons living in higher SES groups within the country (Phinney, Cantu, & Kurtz, 1997; Twenge & Crocker, 2002). Before the year 2000, U.S. Census showed that 34.5 million American citizens were living in poverty and fighting against the negative effects that this lifestyle and low social class entails (e.g., little or no health insurance, negative social stigmatisms, poor health, or increased occurrence of physical and substance abuse) (Buckner, Bassuk, Weinreb, & Brooks, 1999; APA, 2004). Data has shown that boys growing up in poverty are at a higher risk than girls for negative life outcomes for their affiliation in this social group (Buckner, Bassuk, Weinreb, & Brooks, 1999). Currently, Whites are the largest group living in poverty within the United States but minority groups, such as Mexican-American immigrants, are over represented in the poor populations within the country (APA, 2004).

Latino-Americans and Self-Esteem

In the words of Phinney, Chavira, and Williamson (1992), “The ability to function normally in two cultures requires interaction with people outside one’s own group, those who favor separation face added stress and may demonstrate lower self-esteem in situations that require interaction with the dominant culture” (p. 302). Some other factors that have been found to effect the self-esteem of Hispanic Americans include the fact that Latino females have lower self-esteem than males, academic achievement and GPA scores positively correlate with the esteem of Hispanic-American adolescents, the collectivistic goals and actions of the Latino culture helps protect esteem some from prejudice and stereotypes, and a strong commitment and ethnic identity to the Latino membership has been shown to be quite a strong predictor of Latino self-esteem (Phinney, Chavira, & Williamson, 1992; Mezulis, Abramson, Hyde, & Hankin, 2004).

ACCULTURATION

Cultural transition is a process that begins at the time individuals or families arrive in a new country and end when they feel comfortable enough in the new country/culture (e.g., with a new language and values) to relinquish some of their indigenous cultural values and become acculturated to the host culture (Baptiste, p. 342).

Though an official definition for “culture” has been elusive within the field of psychology, it has been defined as the interaction between an individual and the social world around him or her and an identity of membership within that ethnic group (Lafromboise, Coleman, & Gerton, 1993; Hall, Nagayama, Andra, DeGarmo, Sue, & Stephens, 2005). A culturally competent person is presumed to be one who knows, appreciates, and internalizes the basic beliefs of a given culture while behaving in socially sanctioned ways, observing religious rituals, and communicating clearly in the given language of the group (Farver, Narang, & Ghadha, 2002; LaFromboise, Coleman, & Gerton, 1993). Children are acculturated by their parents’ instruction and role modeling, by their interaction with other persons within their ethnic communities, as well as interacting with the dominant cultural group (Farver, Narang, & Ghadha, 2002). Research by Gurung and Mehta (2001) has shown that persons who felt the most “ethnic” and aligned with their culture had the highest self-esteem and overall sense of personal well-being. Still, Phinney, Canut, and Kurtz (1997) demonstrated that ethnic minorities within America are uncertain as to what “being American” means as many figure that their color and culture eliminates them from this grand social category.

Acculturation occurs when someone moves to a new country or already coexists in a multicultural environment where one’s culture is different from the dominant culture of the area, such as what happens when Mexican immigrants move to the United States (Farver, Narang, & Ghadha, 2002). This process is necessary not only for immigrants to the country but is common for persons born within the U.S. but whose parents or grandparents were natives of a different country than America (LaFromboise, Coleman, & Geton, 1993). Here, Mexican-American persons must modify their actions, attitudes, and language, as well as some beliefs and values, in order to achieve a comfortable existence between the two distinct cultures (Sharma & Kerl, 2002; Farver, Narang, & Ghadha, 2002). The process of acculturation can take a very long time to achieve which can cause chronic strain that has been linked with negative effects to self-esteem (Krause, Bennett, & Van Tran, 1989). Even Mexican-Americans who have lived within the USA for many generations can face acculturation problems within their direct environments of work opportunities, socioeconomic status, educational level, and location of residence (Sharma & Kerl, 2002).

Much research has shown that the level of control a body feels they possess with such problems of acculturation can greatly mediate the negative effects of acculturation stress (LaFromboise, Coleman, & Gerton, 1993). As it is, acculturation has been directly linked to such matters of personal health, financial stress, and fragile family relations (Krause, Bennett, & Van Tran, 1989). The common assumption, according to LaFromboise, Coleman, and Gorton (1993), “is that living in two cultures is psychologically undesirable because managing the complexity of dual reference points generates ambiguity, identity confusion, and normlessness” (p.395). Study results are still mixed as to whether someone can be highly acculturated but still have a strong ethnic identity (Farver, Narang, & Ghadha, 2002). Many social psychologists believe that if a person is able to find a way to be grounded comfortably in both cultures than he or she will be able to balance his or her cultural identities without much negative stress (LaFromboise, Coleman, & Gerton, 1993).

Some of the more recent studies within the field have shown that one’s family dynamics have the greatest influence on psychological adjustment, ethnic identity, and acculturation (Farver, Narang, & Ghadha, 2002). Here, parents and elders have a strong influence on how their minority youth socialize and adapt in the acculturation process while reinforcing and modeling the beliefs, traditions and values of their home culture. One of the models of greatest import are how adults within the culture interact with, and their attitudes towards, persons from the dominant culture (Phinney, Cantu, & Kurtz, 1997). As persons in the minority often face discrimination, children within this ethnic group will be greatly influenced by whether or not their elders are fazed by the minority label given by the dominant society (Hall, Nagayama, Andra, DeGarmo, Sue, & Stephens, 2005).

A common problem for acculturated families is that children and adults often become acculturated at different rates with children often adapting more quickly (Lau, McCabe, Yeh, Garland, Wood, & Hough, 2005). This difference of adaptation has been known to cause much discord within ethnic minority families as children tend to learn English more quickly, form cross-racial relations with more of an open mind, and adopt their lifestyle preferences and values to the American way of life more readily than that of older persons within the family (Farver, Narang, & Ghadha, 2002; Lau, McCabe, Yeh, Garland, Wood, & Hough, 2005). This acculturation gap is further hindered by the fact that children were forced to emigrate but once they arrive in America, their preferences differ greatly in what they what for their lives in the U.S. and what their families would desire they maintain of their native culture (Baptiste, 1993). Immigrant parents thus report frustration and anger at what they refer to as “rearing children in two cultures” (Farver, Narang, & Ghadha, 2002). Unfortunately, this intergenerational difference noticed in the acculturation gaps has been found to be a risk factor for poor maladjustment of young Hispanic Americans with behavioral problems, low self-esteem, conduct disorder, and academic conflicts (Lau, McCabe, Yeh, Garland, Wood, & Hough, 2005). An important point is explained by the following:

In the U.S., many immigrant parents perceive their authority to have been circumscribed and report feeling that they function less as parents and more as puppets, dependent upon their children to talk for them in situations requiring English fluency and for much of their interaction with society. As a result, they express feeling that children are more in charge of them than they of the children. For fathers, in particular, the reversal of roles and concomitant loss of absolute authority over children is galling. Many express regret that parental authority seemed not as important to children in the U.S. as in their native country (Baptiste, 1993, p. 348).

The act of emigrating to a new land can be a troublesome or exciting experience, depending on the family member in question and the reason for moving (Farver, Narang, & Ghadha, 2002). This action is confounded by the fact that some families move together as a large unit while often others will have fathers leave first and then mother and children come later, with extended family often emigrating a year or so after that (Baptiste, 1993). Data has indicated that all family members will go through a grieving process from having left their safe and familiar home country for something foreign.

As Hispanic families are known for their interdependent and supportive family networks, this continual separation of ranks can be sorely felt. However, the tradition of interdependence within the Hispanic family will often clash with the independent culture of America and its values (Sharma & Kerl, 2002; Brehm, Kassin, & Fein, 2005). Sometimes connections or family members are already living in the “new country” and can help the newcomers more quickly learn about the new culture, as well as providing informational assistance on language and governmental programs that might help the family settle in more quickly (Baptiste, 1993). The Hispanic connections or extended family within the new culture can have a negative effect as the emigrated family may become dependent on their assistance and they do not interact much directly with the new environment, inhibiting their acculturation progress. More problems arise as there is dissonance among family members who become acclimated more quickly to the new culture and those who acculturate the fastest are often chastised for becoming “too American” and “forgetting who they are and where they came from” (Baptiste, 1993).

Theories of Acculturation

Clinical experiences with immigrant families have shown that those whose native cultures are similar to the new culture, whose members are more than marginally knowledgeable and familiar with the new culture and language, who possess skilss that are transferable and marketable, are better able to cope with the usual and expected changed resulting from emigration (Baptist, 1993) p.353.

Berry and his colleagues developed a famous model of acculturation that argued that people make one of four choices for acculturation: integration (person is able to partake in activities with the dominant culture while maintaining his or her relationships and identity in the native culture), assimilation (one possesses a strong identity to just his or her native culture and severs ties with the other culture), separation (individual rejects the host culture and maintains reliance to native culture), or marginalization (person rejects both the host and native culture) (LaFromboise, Coleman, & Gerton, 1993; Farver, Narang, & Ghadha, 2002; Gurung & Mehta, 2001). Following are other theories regarding the process of acculturation:

The communication theory: the level of acculturation acquired is dependent upon the language proficiency with the interpersonal, intrapersonal, and mass media communications in conjunction with the general language of the dominant culture.

Padilla’s theory of cultural awareness and ethnic loyalty: a person’s preference and pride for the ethnic identity, cultural heritage, and language of his or her native culture will determine his or her level of achieved acculturation.

The Socioanalytic Model states that a person’s unique personality will either facilitate or restrict the learning or expression of culturally appropriate behaviors per a given situation.

The Alternation Model allows for an individual to understand and behave according to two distinct cultures without compromising one’s sense of ethnic identity.

The Fusion Model postulates that all different cultures within an area such as America will fuse together in a collaboration of political, geographic, and economic space to form a completely new culture (LaFromboise, Coleman, & Gerton, 1993).

Sherman, Stroessner, Conrey, & Azam (2005) recently stated that prejudice toward one’s minority affiliation may influence the manner in which one interprets the social data in a given environment. Overall, though, research by Farver, Narang, and Ghadha (2002) has indicated that of all the above acculturation modes, integration appears to be the best choice for strong psychological adapting in the acculturation process and the choice of marginalization for acculturating produced the most negative effects on subject’s self-esteem. Nonetheless, LaFromboise, Coleman, and Gorton (1993) speculate that personality characteristics need to be considered for their unique effect on acculturation competence. Morever, Farver, Narang, and Ghadha (2002) have stated that the greater the difference between the new culture and one’s native cutlure, the greater the stress encountered, as well as the worse psychological functioning of the individual. Lastly, Phinney, Chavira, & Williamson (1992) believe that later generations of immigrants, or older citizens, will have a hard time letting go of their strong affiliation to the native culture they have known for so long while one’s social class may play a part in the choice of acculturation mode as middle-class persons seem to favor assimilation. Irregardless,

Cultural Differences

There are many cultural differences that will play a part in the adaptation of Mexican citizens to the American, dominantly-White, culture (Sue & Sue, 2003). For instance, Latino culture precedents that family and community are key to survival and happiness while America tends to focus on the achievements of the individual beyond all others (Brehm, Kassin, & Fein, 2005). Mexican-American families maintain that family loyalty and unity are highly valued and self-sacrifice for the betterment of the group is not unusual (Castillo, Conoley, & Brossart, 2004). Parents and elders are respected with great power and authority over the family with children knowing that they must honor and obey the wishes of their elders; men are viewed as the patriarch of the family unit (Baptiste, 1993; Sue & Sue, 2003). Women, conversely, are expected to be the caretaker of the family, self-sacrificing always, and submissive to male partners (Castillo, Conoley, & Brossart, 2004). Latino families also include a wide extended family of close friends, grandparents, aunts, uncles, and cousins (Sharma & Kerl, 2002). Partly due to their strong, primarily Catholic, faith, Hispanics enjoy having large families (Sue & Sue, 2003).

Even more differences in the typical American culture and that of the Latinos exist. For example, Americans tend to focus on future goals or special events while Hispanics enjoy living in the present and valuing traditions of the past (Sharma & Kerl, 2002). Additionally, while most native Americans have their individual faiths they still prefer to visit knowledgeable medical facilities when they are ill; Mexican descendents, however, often rely on home remedies, family care, Espiritismo (reliance on help from saints or spirits of deceased relatives), or curanderos--special faith healers within the community, when they are ill or troubled (Borravo & Jenkins, 2003; Baez, 2001; Applewhite, 1995). These beliefs and practices affect the lives of Mexican-Americans greatly as many illegal immigrants will not seek medical attention for fear of deportation and male members often will not divulge sickness as it is admitting personal weakness which is against the machismo role of their culture (Sue & Sue, 2003).

Mexican-American Research

Different studies have produced some interesting data regarding Hispanic Americans within the United States. Such as the fact that the level of acculturation has been found to be extremely important to the occupational and career goals, expectations, and opportunities for this group within the country (McWhirter, Hackett, & Bandalos, 1998). One’s ethnic identity appears to be directly affected by one’s level of acculturation for Mexican-Americans (Castillo, Conoley, & Brossart, 2004). Statistics show that Mexican-Americans have the lowest high school and college completion rates of any group within the country with females apparently feeling there are distinct barriers to any occupational or educational goals for themselves (Flores & O’Brien, 2002; McWhirter, Hackett, & Bandalos, 1998). Additionally, LaFromboise, Coleman, and Gorton (1993) found that youth who could develop bicultural skills within the US were better adjusted, less likely to have academic or family problems, and drug usage was less likely. These results are not too surprising as Farver, Narang, and Ghadha (2002) indicated that Mexican-American parents who had strong ethnic identities were better able to role model positive bicultural skills to their children. More so, Schmitt and Allik (2005) established that persons from collectivistic groups experience higher levels of self-liking and acceptance. Still, Latinos do not feel connected to the “American” identity and this part of their existence has no contribution to Hispanic self-esteem whatsoever (Phinney, Cantu, & Kurtz, 1997). Ultimately, two more interesting facts show that older Mexican-Americans experience the most stress in acculturation and, sadly, Hispanics are the least likely of any group in the United States to obtain higher level skilled jobs in their lifetime (Krause, Bennett, & Van Tran, 1989; McWhirter, Hackett, & Bandalos, 1998).

Language Acculturation

According to LaFromboise, Coleman, and Gerton (1993), communication ability refers to one’s ability to communicate ideas, needs, and feelings to other members of a given culture in both verbal and nonverbal manners, with language competency being a major factor to achieving bicultural competence. Language acculturation refers to the ability to speak the dominant language of a given location, such as Mexican-Americans speaking English well on top of speaking their native Spanish (Krause, Bennett, & Van Tran, 1989). Research on language acculturation shows that relying more on the English language produces more psychological stress on Mexican-Americans but decreases financial stress in older immigrants and increase one’s feelings of personal control and may bolster self-esteem while reliance on the Spanish language producing feelings of less personal control and self-worth. Negy & Snyder (1997) found that Mexican-American wives experienced greater marital satisfaction and harmony when they became more acculturated with wives who did not learn English being unhappy and feeling isolated and alienated in their environment. Mexican-American immigrants who are less acculturated will have trouble communicating outside their own group and will then have less diverse relationships, with this acculturation stress often producing a decrease in self-esteem (Phinney, Cantu, & Kurtz, 1997). And interestingly, an inverse relationship has been found between the length of time a Mexican-American lives in the United States and their use of the Spanish language and the level of their ethnic pride (LaFromboise, Coleman, & Gerton, 1993).

 

Further, Mexican-American children often learn English in school and thus learn the dominant language more quickly than the adults in their ethnic group; this often creates problems and tension within the family as children are frequently asked to translate for their parents out in the English-speaking society which upsets the traditional Latino family roles (Lau, McCabe, Yeh, Garland, Wood, & Hough, 2005; Sue & Sue, 2003). It is intriguing, though, that Mexican-American boys and not girls who prefer the English language experience higher self-esteem and senses of personal control (Krause, Bennett, & Van Tran, 1989). Additionally, it has been learned that students who participate in bilingual programs will perform better on credits attempted and earned and GPAs in school than students who work only in monolingual programs while another study learned that a subject’s proficiency in a second language was conversely related to his or her increased fear of losing one’s ethnic identity (LaFromboise, Coleman, & Gerton, 1993).

Ultimately, the following comment sums up the topics inherent within this paper:

Taken as a whole, these findings suggest that the acculturation process is very complex and that failure to be assimilated into the host culture can exert both positive as well as negative effects on the psychological well-being of Mexican-American immigrants (Krause, Bennett, & Van Tran, 1989, p. 326).

DISCUSSION

Considering all of the above information, it seems necessary for a new study to be created and completed regarding how one’s level of acculturation, as measured by level of competency of the primary English language, effects the self-esteem of Mexican-American immigrants. It would appear that completing such a proposed research study may provide pivotal data regarding how to help Mexicans to better emigrate to the United States without high levels of psychological dissonance, as well as indicating possible strategies that American citizens could employ to aide these persons in their adaptation to the new country.

This author has yet to study how to create research studies nor their execution, but she can provide some starting points for this needed future study. For example, much research has shown the strength of using the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES) to assess global self-esteem, as well as the fact that this test has already been translated into numerous other languages and takes only about 2 minutes for study participants to complete (Schmitt & Allik, 2005). Using the RSES would monitor the subject’s level of self-esteem necessary for the case study at hand and it has been shown to have high internal reliability and is proven to be a valid measure of self-esteem (Gurung & Mehta, 2001). Next, such a study would need to assess the acculturation level of the Mexican-American immigrants of varying ages. Krause, Bennett, and Van Tran (1989) stated that whatever acculturation test applied with this population, it should always include scales for language use, observance of Mexican-American holidays, national origin, and preferences for elements of Mexican-American culture, like music or food. Therefore, it would seem sensible to use the Acculturation Rating Scale for Mexican Americans, or ARSMA, which can classify participants as “Very Mexican”, “Mexican-Oriented”, “True Bicultural”, “Anglo-Oriented”, or “Very Anglicized” in only 20 questions (Sharma & Kerl, 2002)! The ARSMA would be a good instrument to use as it also has high internal consistency, though some have criticized the tool for indicating more the socialization levels of its subjects rather than indicating acculturative stress or bicultural conflicts (Negy & Snyder, 1997). A newer form of the ARSMA has been created, being the ARSMA-II which has 30 items and can assess participants on their association with and identity to Anglo or Mexican cultures on two separate subscales (Castill, Conoley, & Brossart, 2004; Flores & O’Brien, 2002). According to Flores and O’Brien (2002), both the ARSMA and the ARSMA-II are widely used to measure Mexican-American acculturation and has been proven to be a valid and reliable measuring tool. The last suggestion for this future study would be to compare these findings with the Short Acculturation Scale which has been shown to be a useful quick tool for assessing levels of acculturation to the dominant culture for Mexican- Americans (McWhirter, Hackett, & Bandalos, 1998).

Using such tools as indicated above should be a great start to such an important future research study. This author hopes that she will have the opportunity to complete part of this proposed study as her Master’s Integrative Project with Capella University in one year’s time.

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

REFERENCES

APA, (2004). Resolution on poverty and socioeconomic status. Retrieved from the World Wide Web on September 6, 2004 from: http://www.apa.org/pi/urban/povres.html.

Applewhite, S. L. (1995). Curanderismo: Demystifying the health beliefs and practices of elderly. Health & Social Work, 20 (4), 247-253.

Baez, A. (2001). Complementary spiritual beliefs in the Latino community: The interface with psychotherapy. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 71 (4), 408-415.

Baptiste, D.A., Jr. (1993). Immigrant families, adolescents, and acculturation: Insights for therapists. Marriage & Family Review, 19, 341-363.

Bornholt, L.J. (2005). Aspects of self-knowledge about activities: An integrated model of self-concepts. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 21 (3), 156-164.

Borrayo, E. A. & Jenkins, S. R. (2003). Feeling frugal: Socioeconomic status, acculturation, and cultural health beliefs among women of Mexican descent. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 9 (2), 197-206.

Brehm, S.S., Kassin, S., & Fein, S. (2005). Social Psychology (6th ed.). New York: Houghton Mifflin Company.

Buckner, J. C., Basuck, E. I., Weinreb, L. F., & Brooks, M. G. (1999). Homelessness and its relation to the mental health and behavior of low-income school-age children. Developmental Psychology, 35, 246-257.

Cassidy, J., Aikins, J.W., & Chernoff, J.J. (2003). Children’s peer selection: Experimental examination of the role of self-perceptions. Developmental Psychology, 39 (3), 495-508.

Castillo, L.G., Conoley, C.W., & Brossart, D.F. (2004). Acculturation, White marginalization, and family support as predictors of perceived distress in Mexican American female college students. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 51 (2), 151-157.

Cauce, A.M., Domenech-Rodriguez, M., Paradise, M., Cochran, B.N., Shea, J.M., srebik, D., & Baydar, N. (2002).Cultural and contextual influences in mental health help seeking: A focus on ethnic minority youth. Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70 (1), 44-55.

Choi, I., Nisbett, R.E., & Norenzayan, A. (1999). Causal attribution across cultures: Variation and universality. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 47-63.

Conway, L.G. & Schaller, M. (2005). When authorities’ commands backfire: Attribution about consensus and effects on deviant decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89 (3), 311-326.

Crocker, J. & Wolfe, C. T. (2001). Contingencies of self-worth. Psychological

Review, 96, 608-630.

De, D., Carsten, K.W., & van Knippenberg, D. (2005). The possessive self as a barrier to conflict resolution: Effects of mere ownership, process accountability, and self- concept clarity on competitive cognitions and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89 (3), 345-357.

Farver, J.M., Narang, S.K. & Bhadha, B.R. (2002). East meets west: Ethnic identity, acculturation and conflicts in Asian Indian families. Journal of Family Psychology, 16 (3), 338-350.

Flores, L.Y. & O’Brien, K.M. (2002). The career development of Mexican American adolescent women: A test of Social Cognitive Career Theory. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 49 (1), 14-27.

Gest, S.D., Domitrovich, C.E., & Welsh, J.A. (2005). Peer academic reputation in elementary school: Associations with changes in self-concept and academic skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97 (3), 337-346.

Gramzow, R.H.& Gaertner, L. (2005). Self-esteem and favoritism toward novel in- groups: The self as an evaluative base. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88 (5), 801-815.

Gray-Little, B. & Hafdahl, A. R. (2000). Factors influencing racial comparisons of self- esteem: A quantitative review. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 26-54.

Gurung, R.A.R. & Mehta, V. (2001). Relating ethnic identity, acculturation, and attitudes toward treating minority clients. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 7 (2), 139-151.

Hall, G.C., Nagayama, A.L., DeGarmo, D.S., Sue, S. & Stephens, K.A. (2005). Ethnicity, culture, and sexual aggression: Risk and protective factors. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73 (5), 830-840.

Hewitt, P.L., Flett, G.L., Sherry, S.B., Habke, M., Parkin, M., Lam, R.W., McMurtry, B., Ediger, E., Fairlie, P., & Stein, M.B. (2003). The interpersonal expression of perfection: Perfectionistic self-presentation and psychology distress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84 (6), 1303-1325.

Hill, C.E., Gelso, C.J., & Mohr, J.J. (2000). Client concealment and self-presentation in therapy: Comment on. Psychological Bulletin, 126 (4), 495-500.

Hoza, B., Gerdes, A.C., Hinshaw, S.P., Arnold, L.E., Pelham, W.E., Molina, B.S.G., Abikoff, H.B., Epstein, J.N., Greenhill, L.L., Hechtman, L, Odgert, C., Swanson, J.M., & Wigal, T. (2004). Self-perceptions of competence in children with ADHD and comparison children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72 (3), 382-391.

Kersting, K. (2003). Religion and spirituality in the treatment room. Monitor on Psychology, 34 (11); Retrieved from the World Wide Web on September 7, 2004 from: http://www.apa.org/monitor/dec03/religion.html.

Krause, N., Bennett, J., & Van Tran, T. (1989). Age differences in the acculturation process. Psychology and Aging, 4 (3), 321- 332.

LaFromboise, T., Coleman, H.L., & Gerton, J. (1993). Psychological impact of Biculturalism: Evidence and Theory. Psychological Bulletin, 114 (3), 395-412.

Lau, A.S., McCabe, K.M., Yeh, M., Garland, A.F., Wood, P.A., & Hough, R.L. (2005). The Acculturation Gap-Distress Hypothesis among high-risk Mexican American families. Journal of Family Psychology, 19 (3), 367-375.

Levy, B.R., Slade, M.D., Kunkel, S.R., & Karl, S.V. (2002). Longevity increased by positive self-perceptions of aging. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83 (2), 261-270.

McWhirter, E.H., Hackett, G., & Bandalos, D.L. (1998). A causal model of education plans and career expectations of Mexican American high school girls. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 45 (2), 166-181.

Mezulis, A.H., Abramson. L.Y., Hyde, J.S., & Hankin, B.L. (2004). Is there a universal positivity bias in attributions?: A meta-analytic review of individual developmental and cultural differences in the self-serving attribution bias. Psychological Bulletin, 130 (5), 711-747.

Negy, C. & Snyder, D.K. (1997). Ethnicity and acculturation: Assessing Mexican American couples’ relationships using the Marital Satisfaction Inventory-- Revised. Psychological Assessment, 9 (4), 414-421.

Phinney, J.S., Chavira, V., & Williamson, L. (1992). Acculturation attitudes and self- esteem among high school and college students. Youth & Society, 23 (3), 299- 312.

Phinney, J.S., Cantu, C.L., & Kurtz, D.A. (1997). Ethnic and American identity as predictors of self-esteem among African Americans, Latino and White adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 26 (2), 165-186.

Schmitt, D.P. & Allik, J. (2005). Simultaneous administration of the Rosenberg Self- Esteem Scale in 53 nations: Exploring the universal and culture-specific features of global self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89 (4), 623- 642.

Schwartz, J.P., Waldo, M., & Daniel, D. (2005). Gender-role conflict and self-esteem: Factors associated with partner abuse in court-referred men. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 6 (2), 109-113.

Shapka, J.D. & Keating, D.P. (2005). Structure and change in self-concept during adolescence. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 37 (2), 83-96.

Sharma, P. & Kerl, S.B. (2002). Suggestions for psychologists working with Mexican American individuals and families in health care settings. Rehabilitation Psychology, 47 (2), 230-239.

Sherman, J.W., Stroessner, S.J., Conrey, F.R., & Azam, O.A. (2005). Prejudice and stereotype maintenance processes: Attention, attribution, and individuation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89 (4), 607-622.

Sue, D. W. & Sue, D. (2003). Counseling the culturally diverse: Theory and Practice (4th ed.). New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc..

Twenge, J. M. & Campbell, W. K. (2001). Age and birth cohort differences in self- esteem: A cross-temporal meta-analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 321-344.

Twenge, J.M. & Crocker, J. (2002). Race and self-esteem: Meta-analyses comparing Whites, Blacks, Hispanics, Asians, and American Indians and comment on. Psychological Bulletin, 128 (3), 371-408.

Wood, J.V., Heimpel, S.A., Newby-Clark, I.R., & Ross, M. (2005). Snatching defeat from the jaws of victory: Self-esteem differences in the experience and anticipation of success. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89 (5), 764-780.

Enter supporting content here

 
 
Website encompassing the passions and experiences of Sydni Grauberger

moonkitty.gif